Skip to main content

COMMENTARY

Int J Public Health, 12 March 2021

Commentary: Some Social, Psychological, and Political Factors That Undermine Compliance With COVID-19 Public Health Measures

Kathleen D. Magnus
Kathleen D. Magnus*
  • University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Hamburg, Germany

Mask-wearing, social distancing, and hand hygiene have proven effective in mitigating the spread of the COVID-19 virus. However, despite widespread campaigns to inform the public, compliance with these measures has been less than optimal in many Western countries. Various groups, including anti-vaxxers, conspiracy theorists, self-proclaimed “free thinkers,” and far-right extremists, have protested against public health regulations designed to contain the COVID-19 virus. Unlike the opposition to lockdowns, which is often based on competing rational concerns, the resistance to simple practices like mask-wearing defies scientific reason. Insufficient or false information may explain some of this resistance, but there are a number of psychological, social, and political factors that contribute to it as well.

In what follows, I briefly discuss some of the cognitive biases and social dynamics which may lead people to oppose public health efforts to reduce the spread of the COVID-19 virus. In addition, I draw attention to how these psychological and social tendencies have been exploited by politically motivated groups, especially right-wing populists who stand to benefit when people suffer poor health (1, 2). Although public health officials may not be able to intervene directly in the political sphere, they can take some steps to increase compliance with COVID-19 mitigating measures and to counter the political forces that seek to undermine them.

A number of cognitive biases may lead even the most reasonable people to disregard public health recommendations at least some of the time. For example, a particularly prevalent bias, the “optimism bias,” may incline people to underestimate their own chances of getting sick (3). In other words, even people who recognize the real risk of catching COVID-19 may maintain a strong belief that they will not catch it. (“Sure, it’s a problem, but I won’t get sick.”) In some cases, this belief is so strong that people neglect to wear a mask or to social distance even when these precautions are clearly warranted. This bias may come into play regardless of an individual’s social situation or political affiliations; however, it becomes particularly problematic when combined with the exceptionalism promulgated by certain right-wing populist groups. By encouraging people to see themselves as belonging to a special, “protected” group, right-wing populist leaders transform the individual defense mechanism (“I won’t get sick”) into an issue of greater social relevance (“it’s not our problem,” “it’s an urban issue,” etc.). This happened in the United States last spring. After right-wing populist media propagated the idea that the COVID-19 virus was “a city problem,” many local and state governments failed to implement adequate measures to prevent the spread of the virus in rural areas (4).

The situation is similar with respect to the “normalcy bias.” Given a strong psychological preference for normalcy, people are reluctant to prepare for disaster scenarios or to respond to them when they occur (5). This bias explains why some people refuse to evacuate their homes when authorities warn them of approaching hurricanes, floods, or forest fires, and in the context of the current health crisis, it may also explain the reluctance some people have to protect themselves against COVID-19. Again this bias may affect any individual regardless of social situation or political affiliations; however, it plays especially well into the hands of right-wing populist leaders and conspiracy theorists who want to convince people that the virus poses no serious threat. Because people are already inclined to doubt that abrupt, disastrous change can happen, many are quick to follow right-wing populist leaders who downplay the virus and flout the advice of public health experts.

Another reason people may choose to ignore public health regulations stems from the fundamentally social nature of the human psyche. People develop their identities in relation to others, and their own judgments are heavily influenced by what others think (6). Moreover, the desire to belong to the “in-group” generates a will to conform that often leads to false conclusions (7), and this desire has been shown to influence people’s decisions about their own health (8). In some societies this tendency to conform has translated into a unified response to mitigate the spread of COVID-19, but in other countries it has been used to undermine measures that are meant to reduce contagion. Right-wing populists in the United States and Europe serve as prime examples of this dynamic. By turning the refusal to wear masks into a symbol of group identity, they undermine health experts, encourage citizens to risk their health, and stoke social division all at once.

The public health community may counter these and other psychological, social, and political forces through short and long-term initiatives. Clear, consistent, fact-based information goes a long way to keep irrational judgments and behaviors in check. However, given the deluge of misinformation that circulates through various media outlets, public health information campaigns must be vigorous and unrelenting. Even so, they will not suffice to change the behavior of people who are committed to their own cognitive biases and deeply invested in the belief that the opinions of their group are superior to all others. For this reason, long-term programs that teach people how to recognize and reflect upon their own cognitive biases and psychological vulnerabilities must be developed. In the meantime, public health officials must acknowledge the powerful social, psychological, and political forces that undermine compliance, especially those stemming from right-wing populists who have a long-standing record of opposing public health measures (9, 10). Moreover, given the urgency of the current health crisis, officials should not hesitate to implement regulations with harsh penalties for non-compliance. This may be the only way to avoid more severe restrictions such as lockdowns, which are bound to provoke even greater resistance.

Author Contributions

The author confirms being the sole contributor of this work and has approved it for publication.

Conflict of Interest

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

1. Gugushvili, A, Koltai, J, Stuckler, D, and McKee, M. Votes, populism, and pandemics. Int J Publ Health (2020). 65(6):721–2. doi:10.1007/s00038-020-01450-y PubMed Abstarct

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. McKee, M, Gugushvili, A, Koltai, J, and Stuckler, D. Are populist leaders creating the conditions for the spread of COVID-19? Comment on “A scoping review of populist radical right parties’ influence on welfare policy and its implications for population health in Europe”. Int J Health Pol Manag (2020). 10.34172/ijhpm.2020.124

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Bavel, JJV, Baicker, K, Boggio, PS, Capraro, V, Cichocka, A, Cikara, M, et al. Using social and behavioural science to support COVID-19 pandemic response. Nat Hum Behav (2020). 4:460. doi:10.1038/s41562-020-0884-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Brownstein, R. Coronavirus is dividing blue cities from their red states. CNN (2020). Available from: https://edition.cnn.com/2020/03/31/politics/red-states-blue-cities-coronavirus/index.html (Accessed April 1, 2020).

Google Scholar

5. Omer., H, and Alon., N. The continuity principle: a unified approach to disaster and trauma. Am J Community Psychol (1994). 22:273–87. 10.1007/BF02506866

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Asch, S. Studies of independence and conformity: I. A minority of one against a unanimous majority. Psychol Monogr (1956). 70:1–70. doi:10.1037/h0093718

7. Janis, IL. Groupthink: psychological studies of policy decisions and fiascoes. Boston: Houghton Mifflin Company (1982).

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Attwell, K, Meyer, SB, and Ward, PR. The social basis of vaccine questioning and refusal: a qualitative study employing Bourdieu’s concepts of ‘capitals’ and ‘habitus’. Int J Environ Res Publ Health (2018). 15(5):1044. 10.3390/ijerph15051044

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Lockwood, M. Right-wing populism and the climate change agenda: exploring the linkages. Environ Polit (2018). 27:4. 10.1080/09644016.2018.1458411

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. McKenna, M. The Plague years: how the rise of right-wing nationalism is jeopardizing the world’s health. The New Republic (2019). Posted April 1, 2019. Available from: https://newrepublic.com/article/153264/rise-right-wing-nationalism-jeopardizing-world-health (Accessed April 1, 2019).

Google Scholar

Keywords: COVID-19, right-wing populism, cognitive bias, compliance, public health restrictions, groupthink

Citation: Magnus KD (2021) Commentary: Some Social, Psychological, and Political Factors That Undermine Compliance With COVID-19 Public Health Measures. Int J Public Health 66:603944. doi: 10.3389/ijph.2021.603944

Received: 08 September 2020; Accepted: 02 March 2021;
Published: 12 March 2021.

Edited by:

Nino Kuenzli, Swiss Tropical and Public Health Institute, Switzerland

Copyright © 2021 Magnus. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Kathleen D. Magnus, ka.magnus@uke.de

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.