Skip to main content

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Int J Public Health, 20 June 2022
This article is part of the Special Issue THE IMPACT OF THE COVID-19 PANDEMIC ON MENTAL HEALTH View all 60 articles

The Kids Are Alright (?). Infants’ Development and COVID-19 Pandemic: A Cross-Sectional Study

Eleonora Ferrari&#x;Eleonora Ferrari1Lucia Palandri
&#x;Lucia Palandri1*Laura Lucaccioni,,Laura Lucaccioni2,3,4Giovanna Talucci,Giovanna Talucci1,2Erica PassiniErica Passini3Viola TrevisaniViola Trevisani3Elena RighiElena Righi1
  • 1Department of Biomedical, Metabolic and Neural Sciences, University of Modena and Reggio Emilia, Modena, Italy
  • 2Pediatric Unit, Department of Medical and Surgical Sciences for Mothers, Children and Adults, University of Modena and Reggio Emilia, Modena, Italy
  • 3Department of Medical and Surgical Sciences for Mothers, Children and Adults, Postgraduate School of Pediatrics, University of Modena and Reggio Emili, Modena, Italy
  • 4Neonatal Intensive Care Unit, Department of Medical and Surgical Sciences for Mothers, Children and Adults, University of Modena and Reggio Emilia, Modena, Italy

Objectives: The study aimed to assess and compare the global development in six-month-old infants before and during the pandemic restrictive social distancing measures.

Methods: This cross-sectional nested study involved infants assessed through the Griffiths Scales of Child Development (GSCD) between September 2019 and April 2021. Infants were classified in a pre-COVID or a COVID group, considering the evaluation date and the restrictive measures in place. GSCD subscales and General Development Scores (GDS) were calculated and compared.

Results: One hundred and four healthy term-born infants were evaluated. GDS in the COVID group (n:70; median: 94; IQR: 90–100) appeared significantly lower than in the pre-COVID group (n:34; median: 98; IQR: 97–103; p < 0.001). Language and personal-social-emotional subareas scores appeared the most affected. A decreasing trend of GDS along with the severity of restriction was observed.

Conclusion: A reduction in infant development scores was observed during pandemic social distancing. Further studies are needed to systematize these findings and to address effective public health policies for infants and families during long-term forced isolation periods.

Introduction

On 11 March 2020, the Italian government imposed a national lockdown to reduce the spread of severe acute respiratory syndrome coronavirus 2 (SARS-CoV-2) [1]. After the pandemic outbreak, a succession of multiple waves took place, leading to the implementation of different restrictive measures in order to curtail the diffusion of the virus. All familial profiles are at risk when drastic changes—such as confinement—occur, especially those with higher susceptibility to socioeconomic factors and previous internal psychological problems [2].

While children’s health is clearly a key investment for public health [3], it is well established that unpredictable adverse events (such as war, natural disasters, and pandemics) have a huge impact on familial relationships and can affect the social and environmental background of a growing child [4]. Epidemics and pandemics, such as COVID-19, bring potential risks to child development in many fields and shape up to be possible “adverse childhood experiences” (ACEs) [5].

Many exogen stressors emerged during COVID-19 [6], threatening families’ financial security and routine, as well as preschoolers’ access to nurseries. Social restrictions caused children to spend time alone with their family nuclei, and newborns’ social and language abilities are known to be strongly influenced by their social environment. Evidence suggests that the amount of speech children hear, lexical diversity, and inclusion of language goals in play have a major role in children’s cognitive, language, and social development t [7].

Assessing the potential impact of social restrictions due to the pandemic on children’s development is an important public health goal. Although the association between isolation due to the COVID-19 pandemic and adverse mental health outcomes has been established in adult and adolescent cohorts [812], few studies [13, 14] have examined the association between changes in psychological functioning and home confinement in pre-school children.

The aim of the present study is to describe the global development as measured by the Griffiths Scales of Child Development (GSCD) [4, 15] of a cohort of six-month-old infants which were evaluated partially before and partially during the COVID-19 pandemic outbreak in periods characterized by different levels of restrictive measures.

Methods

Study Design and Participants

This is a cross-sectional analysis nested on an ongoing prospective cohort study. The main study started in 2019 with the aim of investigating the relationship between exposure to phthalates and anthropometric and neurocognitive development in Italian children during the first 3 years of life. Study subjects were enrolled at birth and follow-up visits were planned at 3, 6, 15, and 36 months. Neurocognitive development evaluation was programmed from the 6 months follow-up, onwards [16].

The study was approved by Area Vasta Emilia Nord Ethics Committee (2018/num715). Informed consent to participation and publication was obtained from caregivers of all individual participants included in the study.

Newborns with mothers of legal age (>18 years old) at delivery, who had an understanding of the Italian language and had a physiological single-child pregnancy were eligible for participation in the main cohort study. Other inclusion criteria were delivery occurred at term (37–41 weeks), giving birth to an appropriate-for-gestational-age (AGA) infant, with an Apgar score >7 at 5 minutes.

Invitation and enrollment took place in a second-level university hospital in Modena (Italy) between March 2019 and October 2020. Sample size calculation was performed for the main study, and it was estimated that about 200 newborns were needed. Therefore, we enrolled 197 mothers and child pairs. Among them, 104 infants completed the global development assessment at 6 months, and this determined the sample size for the present cross-sectional study. The six-month follow-up underwent from September 2019 to April 2021. The first local SARS-CoV-2 case in Italy was diagnosed on 21st February 2020. This unexpected situation gave us the opportunity to describe and compare global development in six-month-old healthy infants submitted to different levels of social restrictions.

Variables

Regarding outcome, the global child development was assessed by the GSCD (third edition) [4, 15] a widely used [1719] specialistic tool exploring five key evolutionary areas: the “Foundations of Learning” subscale (scale A), the “Language and Communication” subscale (scale B), the “Eye and Hand Coordination” subscale (scale C), the “Personal-Social-Emotional” subscale (scale D) and the “Gross Motor” subscale (scale E). Subscale scores and a total score, the “General Development score” (GDS) were calculated. Subscale and GDS raw scores were standardized for sex and age according to GSCD normative scoring tables and guidelines, and standardized “Developmental Quotient (DQ)” scores were produced. In the present study, we used the Italian validated version GSCD questionnaire addressed to 0–12 months children and the normative scoring tables produced for the Italian population [20]. The Griffiths III administration manual [15] lists seven classes for scoring interpretation: Extremely high (DQ ≥ 130), High (DQ = 120–129), Above average (DQ = 110–119), Average (DQ = 90–109), Below average (DQ 80–89), At the limits (DQ = 70–79), Extremely low (DQ ≤ 69). Accordingly, we classified the observed standardized DQ into three categories: Above average (DQ ≥ 110), Average (DQ = 90–109) and Below average (DQ ≤ 89) scores.

GSCD is a play-based test determining the level of development in infants aged 1 month to 5 years and 11 months (71 months) by observing their performance in a variety of standardized activities.

In the present study, a trained team of pediatricians and a psychologist administered the GSCD at the six-month follow-up visit as scheduled in the main longitudinal study. The mean duration of the visit was 1 h 15 and the mean time required for GSCD administration was 45 min. During assessment, the primary focus of specialists is on observing the child interacting and playing with the instruments provided in the GSCD administration kit. After February 2020 due to pandemic restrictions, some of the 6-month follow-up visits were performed online. Staff and mothers were trained to accurately reproduce the procedures and assessments of in-person GSCD visits.

Exposure to the pandemic outbreak was recorded as dichotomous (pre-COVID/COVID) depending on if the evaluation took place before or after 4th March 2020, the date of the first regional “lockdown.” Data on the severity of restriction was registered as categorical data depending on how severe restrictions during the 14 days before the visit were and defined as per local legislation during the observed period. Five levels were defined (none, mild, moderate, strict, very strict) and Figure 1 shows the time period during which they were implement in the study area. Details on each level definition are reported in Supplementary Material.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. Timeline overview of the study process according to the implementation of different restrictive policies. Different patterns represent the severity of the restrictions put in place in Modena province in the study period. Italy, 2019–2021.

Considering that 6-month-old newborns are rapidly evolving, we chose to use a 14-day separation window from the evaluation day in order to intercept short-term impact of social restrictions on infants’ global development. However, since we have not found a consensus in literature on how to define the timing of restriction in light of its possible effects on child’s development, we conducted a sensitivity analysis for the severity of restriction changing cut-off classification. To assess consistency, we conducted sensitivity analysis defining the severity of restriction set up on the period of the visit.

Covariates

According to the theory of the avenues of learning by Ruth Griffiths [21] and its following revisions [4] child development is a complex process resulting from the interactions of multiple environmental, social and familial factors. Major items interacting in the GSCD paradigm of development are familial involvement in a child’s growth and the social-economical-cultural background of the parents. Moreover, unexpected events, such as social changes and restrictions imposed during the COVID-19 pandemic, can affect children’s abilities to adjust to unknown situations. Accordingly, to adjust for potential confounding effects, we collected data on the educational level, work category, and nationality of both parents which were used as proxy descriptors of respectively cultural, economic, and social background of the family. Data on socio-demographic characteristics of caregivers were collected during the enrollment visit and familial involvement was calculated from “Gender stereotypes, educational relations, and infancies” [22] promoted by the Emilia-Romagna region administered to the caregiver of the infant contextually GSCD evaluation. The “Gender stereotypes, educational relations, and infancies” survey for parents is composed of 35 items focusing on parenting themes from a gender equality perspective.

To specifically examine the relationship between the parents’ cultural background and the baby’s cognitive performance according to the GSCD scales, we defined three educational levels (1 = none/primary/middle school; 2 = high school degree; 3 = University or more).

Working groups have been defined according to European Socio-economic Groups (ESeG) [23] classification composed of 9 categories (managers, professionals, technicians and associated professionals employees, small entrepreneurs, clerks and skilled service employees, skilled industrial employees, lower status employees, retired persons, other not employed persons).

Parental involvement levels have been measured as an ordinal variable ranging from 0 to 3 (0 = insufficient, 1 = minimum, 2 = good, 3 = excellent) according to McBride and Mills model [24] and extracting self-reported data from the 15th item (“How would you describe you and your partner in the relationship with your children?”) of the “Gender stereotypes, educational relations, and infancies” survey.

For more information on covariates level definition and administered questionnaires refer to Supplementary Material.

Statistical Methods

Unless otherwise stated, categorical variables were summarized by absolute and relative frequencies. Median and interquartile ranges (IQR) were used to show continuous variables in the paper. Full descriptive statistical analysis including mean, median, minimum–maximum, standard deviation, and IQR for continuous variables may be found in Supplementary Material.

We calculated descriptive statistics and frequency tables to highlight the characteristics of the population under study and we stratified our sample into two groups: pre-COVID and COVID. We applied the χ2 test to measure differences in the distribution of categorical variables in the two groups. The Mann-Whitney U test was used for the comparison of GSCD subscales and GDS scores between Pre-COVID and COVID groups, as data were not normally distributed as assessed by the Shapiro-Wilk normality test.

To assess the GDS time trend locally weighted regression (loess) was used [25]. Linear regression was used to test if the type of restrictions significantly predicted GDS. Type of restriction was entered in the model as an ordinal 5-level variable starting from the least level of restriction to the highest level. By the theory of the avenues of learning by Ruth Griffiths [21] and its following revisions [26] to control for potential confounders the regression model was adjusted for age, nationality, educational level, working status of both parents, and familial involvement level. Age and sex of infants were not included in the model as all GSCD scores are expressed as DQ standardized scores.

Missing data Analysis

Missing data patterns were analyzed and, when possible, a complete-case analysis was performed. Less than 3% of the study participants had missing outcome data (3 for GDS given by 2 missing data in subscale D and E, and 1 in subscale C) all in online visits, while all had data for the exposure of interest (time of visit and severity of restrictions). Given literature considerations on the small percentage of missing data [27, 28], bivariate analysis between exposures and outcomes was performed with complete case analysis.

Regarding covariate, data were complete for mothers and fathers’ nationality. Data on age category, education, and work were missing for, respectively, 30, 22, and 21 (29%, 21%, and 20%) fathers. Data on familial involvement were missing for 24 subjects (23%). The missing data pattern suggests that observations were missing at random (MAR). A complete description of missing data analysis may be found in the Supplementary Material.

Subgroup Analysis and Sensitivity Analyses

Subgroup Analysis

Due to pandemic restrictions, some of the visits (n:35) were performed online. Staff tried to accurately reproduce the procedures and assessments of in-person GSCD visits, yet, given the possible bias in test evaluation during online visits, and given the higher variability of the scores producing some outlier scores, we performed a subgroup analysis on infants visited exclusively in presence. This determined the exclusion of infants evaluated in the most restrictive periods (those classified in strict and very strict severity of restriction levels) but gave us the possibility of exclude from the analyses outliers as well.

Sensitivity Analyses

In multivariable analyses, to minimize the potential for bias and loss of information due to missing data, multiple imputation using chained equations (MICE) was performed and linear regression with and without MICE was calculated [29]. Missing data analysis and multiple imputation approach are described in Supplementary Material.

Finally, we conducted a sensitivity analysis for the severity of restriction changing the timing of cut-off classification. We have not found a consensus in literature on how to define the timing of restriction in light of its possible effects on child’s development. Given that infants are rapidly evolving, in the primary analysis, we defined the type of restrictions more represented in the 14 days prior to visit. However, to assess consistency, we conducted sensitivity analysis defining the severity of restriction set up on the period of the visit.

Data were analyzed using the statistical software IBM SPSS Statistics software, version 27 [30].

The present study was reported according to Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE) guidelines [31].

Results

We analyzed data collected from 104 infants and their parents. 34 infants underwent 6-month evaluations before the pandemic started and 70 after. Table 1 shows the demographic characteristics of the study population, visit method, age, nationality and socio-economic status of both parents, and familial involvement in child growth. Most infants were 6 or 7 months old (86.5%), female and male children were equally represented. 100% of the visits were conducted in person before the pandemic outbreak, while in the “COVID” group, 50% of the visits were conducted online through video call assessment. More than half of the mothers were older than 35 years (64.4%), 92.3% were of Italian nationality and 71.2% had a high educational level (university degree or Ph.D.). Most women worked as professionals (30.8%) or as clerks and skilled service employees (26.0%). For the fathers, more than half of them were older than 35 years (63.5%), 96.2% were of Italian nationality and 46.8% held a degree (49.4% had a high school diploma). Most of them worked as clerks and skilled service employees (31.3%) or professionals (25.3%), a lower percentage of men worked as lower status employees (16.9%) or small entrepreneurs/managers (13.2%). Most families showed an excellent (75.0%) or good (24.2%) involvement in their children’s evolution and growth.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Demographic-socio-economic characteristics of infants and caregivers and familial involvement expressed in n (%). Italy, 2019–2021.

As reported in Figure 2A, both global and single scale standardized scores appeared more widespread in the COVID group than in the pre-COVID group. Further, in this group GSCD scores (median: 94; IQR: 90–100) were significantly lower than those observed in the pre-COVID group (median: 98; IQR: 97–103; Mann-Whitney U test p < 0.001). Differences in score distribution become particularly relevant for subscales B and D (exploring respectively “Language and Communication” and “Personal-social-emotional” areas). Details on subscale score distributions according to period of the assessment are reported in Supplementary Material. As reported in Table 2, a significantly higher number of 6-month infants in the COVID group showed scores below the average, as defined by the normative scoring values for the Italian population [20], both for the global score (22.4% vs. 8.8%, χ2 test p < 0.05) and for the specific scale scores. The main differences were observed for subscales B (“Language and Communication”) where 70% and 26.5% scores below the average were observed respectively in COVID and pre-COVID group (χ2 test p < 0.001) and for scale D (“Personal-social-emotional” area) where 60.3% of the COVID group scores lies below the average while only 5.9% of scores are included in the same category for the pre-COVID group (χ2 test p < 0.001). More details on these findings are reported in Supplementary Material.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. Standardized development quotient scores (DQ) for General Development (GD) and subscales, stratified by evaluation occurring before or after COVID-19 first lockdown. Italy, 2019–2021. Scale A is “Foundations of Learning” scale, Scale B is “Language and Communication” scale, Scale C is “Eye and Hand Coordination” scale, scale D is “Personal-Social-Emotional” scale, scale E is “Gross Motor” scale. Horizontal lines indicate cutoff value of DQ standardized score above and under the average range as calculated for the Italian general population of the same age and sex, according to the GSCD procedure manual [20]. (A) Entire sample. (B) Subgroup analysis excluding online visits.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 2. Standardized development quotient scores (DQ) per specific subscale and for the global development score (GDS). Italy, 2019–2021.

Figure 3A shows the GDS scores time trend. Data show a decreasing trend in GDS in infants evaluated since the start of the pandemic. Locally weighted regression shows a slight increase of scores during the period of least restrictions (White zone) and a rapid decrease with the imposition of more severe local restrictions.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 3. Time trend of standardized development quotient scores (DQ) for General Development (GD). Different coloring represents the severity of restriction occurring 14 days prior to evaluation. Italy, 2019–2021. (A) Entire sample. (B) Subgroup analysis excluding online visits.

Simple linear regression was used to test if the type of restrictions significantly predicted GDS (Table 3). The overall regression was statistically significant (Adj.R2 = 0.237, p < 0.001).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 3. Linear regression models testing if the severity of restriction predicts GDS. Italy, 2019–2021.

To adjust for potential confounders the regression model was adjusted for age, nationality, educational level, working status of both parents, and familial involvement level. The overall regression was statistically significant (Adj.R2 = 0.331, p = 0.003). and the worsening of the type of restrictions continued to be associated to a significant decrease in GDS (p = 0.001) while other covariates did not significantly predict GDS. Finally given the high percentage of missing data in covariate variables (24% of the sample), multiple regression was performed after multiple imputation and pooled data from linear regression from fifty-five imputed datasets using chained equations (MICE) showed how the severity of restriction continued to significantly affect GDS (p < 0.001). Covariates continued to be not significantly associated to GDS.

Subgroup Analysis and Sensitivity Analysis

Given the possible bias in test evaluation during online visits, we performed a subgroup analysis on infants visited exclusively in presence. As reported in Figure 2B results observed in the whole cohort were confirmed: infants assessed during COVID pandemic showed lower scores than those evaluated in pre-COVID period in all scales and especially in sub-scales B and D, respectively Language and Communication and Personal-Social-Emotional subscale. Details on subscale scores for this subgroup of infants are reported in Supplementary Material. Figure 3B shows a similar over-time decreasing pattern as seen with the entire cohort.

Pooled data from multiple linear regression after MICE showed that the level of restriction continues to significantly affect GDS (p = 0.009). Covariates did not significantly predict GDS.

Finally, we conducted a sensitivity analysis for the type of restriction changing the cut-off classification of restriction from defining zones as the most representative restriction during the 14 days prior to visit defining zones based on the restriction rules set up on the period of the visit. Results of regression are similar even if with slightly worse performance and are shown in Table 3.

Discussion

High media and scientific attention raised on the socio-psychological consequences of the COVID-19 pandemic [32, 33] and the World Health Organization (WHO) has identified mental health as an integral component of the COVID-19 response [34].

This pandemic has undermined the sense of security of the population, putting families in economic difficulty and completely changing their daily lives. During quarantine, many children experienced isolation periods and were not allowed to meet friends, relatives and other people outside the household. Since in infants social and language development is widely influenced from social background, it is important to understand the consequences of such changes on newborns.

Using data collected for an ongoing cohort study, we evaluated the general child development, as assessed by Griffiths Scales of Child Development, in six-month-old healthy infants which were assessed partly before the pandemic and partly during the pandemic restrictive social distancing measures. Our results suggest an association between social distancing measures and reduction in Griffiths Scales of Child Development scores as significantly lower scores both in General Development and in subscales were observed in the children assessed during the pandemic period. Scores below-average as reported by GSCD normative scoring tables for the Italian population [20] were observed mainly in “Language and Communication” and “Personal-social-emotional” areas. Time trend analysis showed a decrease in global score over time that seems to follow the severity of social distancing restriction. This last result was observed also when linear regression analyses were performed.

The results of our study must be considered in light of its limitations. Firstly, given the unexpected nature of the pandemic, subject enrollment and sample size were not designed to answer this specific question. The 6-month infants evaluated in this study, actually, were originally enrolled in a longitudinal birth cohort study aimed at investigating the relationship between environmental exposure to phthalates and anthropometric and neurocognitive development in Italian children during the first 3 years of life. The occurrence of the COVD pandemic during the 6-month follow up of the cohort, gave us the opportunity to describe and compare child global development in six-month-old healthy infants exposed to different levels of social restrictions. As the cohort follow up study is still going on, we will have as well the opportunity to follow the changes over time of this specific score. Secondly, familial involvement levels were not assessed directly: a selection of self-reported information collected in the “Gender stereotypes, educational relations, and infancies” survey was used as proxy measures of parental involvement in infant growth. The McBride and Mills Categories of Parental Involvement scheme [24] is the theoretical model that we used to derive familial involvement covariate. According to Ruth Griffiths’ avenues of learning paradigm [21] family engagement, along with relational coherence, self-care, and attachment style, is a primary component determining the child’s social-emotional development. In our study, we analyzed only the first of these aspects, with no contextual analysis of parenting style or of psychological well-being, stress, or depressive symptoms of parents. This shortcoming could have reduced the validity of our findings. Thirdly, given the confinement measures introduced during the pandemic, we did not perform all the GSCD evaluation through visits in person: the online visits represent an unconventional way to administer the Griffiths III Scales of Child Development and it must be noted. However, sensitivity analysis performed excluding online visits showed consistent results. Finally, a possible selection bias should be recognized since it was not possible to verify whether mothers and families who took part in the study differed in their socioeconomic, familial, and demographic characteristics from those who did not choose to participate. The demographic characteristics of the infants and the parents in our sample are congruent with those of the population in the Emilia-Romagna region as for infant sex, parent’s nationality, and parent’s occupational status [35, 36]. However, data we recorded from both mothers and fathers show that they have a higher educational level than the average in the Emilia-Romagna area making our results more applicable in a context with a higher level of education.

In a recent preprint work, Deoni et al. described initial signs of substantial cognitive and performance deficits in infants born during the pandemic and suggested that early development is impaired by the environmental conditions brought on by the pandemic [37]. Early findings of altered temperament in 3-month-old infants are also provided by Provenzi et al. [26] Our results show that most of the examined infants are in line with the national average rates indicative of regular development. Still, a significant reduction of the GDS was recorded (from a median standardized score of 98 to a median standardized score of 93). Overall, we found that subscales evaluating “Language and Communication” and “Personal-social-emotional” areas seem to be most affected by the pandemic outbreak.

This finding is consistent with the analysis of the major developmental milestones of infants [38]: language and social-emotional abilities flourish up to 2 years of age, with external stimuli serving as the primary drivers of such development. In particular, in the “Prespeech” stage of language development, receptive language is crucial, and sound localization skills are substantially improved. During protracted periods of isolation this set of external stimuli risk to become very poor.

On the other hand, it should be noted that during quarantine, some couples or families may have paid greater attention to their babies, spending more time with them and enriching their home environment with more stimuli and this could have, at least partially, counterbalanced the negative effects on infant general development of the social restrictive measures [39, 40].

As for the social and emotional aspects of neurodevelopment, the situation is even more tangled. During lockdown, parents may have experienced low mood and other psychological symptoms [6], which may have influenced their perception of child development. According to a recent study [41], parents tended to be particularly concerned about their children’s development during lockdown, and a “negative influence” on the children’s social-emotional development coming from the parents themselves cannot be excluded.

Considering that a child’s growth is a continuous process constantly influenced by life experiences and environmental background, our results seem to suggest that infants are not immune to the adverse effects of the enacted restriction on cognitive development and mental health.

Our findings also seem to show that the severity of the restrictions negatively affects the infants’ scores, as shown in the results of linear regression: the more the degree of restriction increases, the more the GDS decreases. It is hard to find studies examining specifically the strength of social isolation measures during COVID-19 as a predictor of adverse development outcomes in healthy infants. Therefore, to the best of our knowledge, our paper is the first to describe a preliminary overview of how GDS scores in 6 months old infants decrease over time in relation to the severity of the social restrictions in place. This result must be considered with all its limitations, since, to date and with our dataset, it is difficult to untangle the potential specific factors involved in the decrease of scores over time observed after the pandemic outbreak. Further, it is difficult to define and identify the most susceptible time window potentially affecting child development. Since we have not found a consensus or specific suggestions in literature and, considering that 6-month-old newborns are rapidly evolving, we chose to use a 14-day separation window from the evaluation day in order to intercept short-term impact of social restrictions on infants’ global development. However, a cumulative effect, related to the length of time the infants spent living in the “pandemic era,” could be hypothesized as well. As widely stated, the relevance of familial and socio relationships as environments for early socio-personality development is crucial [38] and, during the pandemic, confinement measures have led to an important social deprivation. The first 1,000 days of a child’s life represent a highly sensitive moment for child development and growth [42] and our findings highlighted that there are specific areas of a child’s development that are more vulnerable than others when infants are affected by confinement measures.

These findings strengthen the need for additional research specifically designed in order to identify and address the specific risk and protective factors potentially involved in adverse developmental outcomes in early preschoolers exposed to isolation periods.

From a public health point of view, it appears important to fill the knowledge gap surrounding the short and long-term impacts of the current pandemic on infant and children’s general development. Public health decision-makers are called to balance the necessity for SARS-CoV-2 spread control and the effect that said measures have on the population. As outlined by Deoni et al. [37] this lack of information slows the development of evidence-based strategies to follow-up sensitive infants or informed guidance for school and daycare reopening.

Data Availability Statement

The data underlying this article will be shared at reasonable request to the corresponding author.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Comitato Etico dell’ Area Vasta Emilia Nord Regione Emilia-Romagna, Italy, with protocol nr: 715/2018. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin.

Author Contributions

All authors have participated in the study and consent to the publication of this manuscript. LP, EF, LL, and ER authors contributed to the study’s conception and design. Material preparation and data collection were performed by LL, GT, EP, VT, and EF and analysis was performed by LP and EF under ER supervision. The first draft of the manuscript was written by EF and LP, all authors commented on previous versions of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

Funding

This study was partially funded by the research grant “University funding grant for interdisciplinary proposals year 2018” emitted by the University of Modena and Reggio Emilia, Italy (Fondo di Ateneo per la Ricerca anno 2018. Progetto di Ricerca interdisciplinare). The authors did not receive support from any organization for the submitted work.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We want to thank the caregivers and infants who participated in this study. We thank residents, physicians, and nurses for supporting inpatient management and data collection.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.ssph-journal.org/articles/10.3389/ijph.2022.1604804/full#supplementary-material

References

1.Decree of the Prime Minister. Further Implementing Provisions of the Decree-Law 23 February 2020, No. 6, with Urgent Measures in Relation to Containment and Management of the Epidemiological Emergency from COVID-19, Applicable throughout the Country. (20A01605). G.U. General Series, No. 64 [in Italian] (2020). Available from: https://www.trovanorme.salute.gov.it/norme/%20dettaglioAtto?id=73643.

Google Scholar

2. Cusinato, M, Iannattone, S, Spoto, A, Poli, M, Moretti, C, Gatta, M, et al. Stress, Resilience, and Well-Being in Italian Children and Their Parents during the COVID-19 Pandemic. Int J Environ Res Public Health (2020) 17(22):8297. doi:10.3390/ijerph17228297

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Clark, H, Coll-Seck, AM, Banerjee, A, Peterson, S, Dalglish, SL, Ameratunga, S, et al. A Future for the World's Children? A WHO-UNICEF-Lancet Commission. Lancet (2020) 395(10224):605–58. doi:10.1016/s0140-6736(19)32540-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Lanfranchi, S, Rea, M, Vianello, R, and Ferri, R. GRIFFITHS III - Griffiths Scales of Child Development: Part I Manual: Overview, Development and Psychometric Properties. Italian Edition. 2nd ed. Florence: Hogrefe (2017).

Google Scholar

5. Petruccelli, K, Davis, J, and Berman, T. Adverse Childhood Experiences and Associated Health Outcomes: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Child Abuse Neglect (2019) 97:104127. doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2019.104127

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Brooks, SK, Webster, RK, Smith, LE, Woodland, L, Wessely, S, Greenberg, N, et al. The Psychological Impact of Quarantine and How to Reduce it: Rapid Review of the Evidence. Lancet (2020) 395(10227):912–20. doi:10.1016/s0140-6736(20)30460-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Davies, C, Hendry, A, Gibson, SP, Gliga, T, McGillion, M, and Gonzalez-Gomez, N. Early Childhood Education and Care (ECEC) during COVID-19 Boosts Growth in Language and Executive Function. Infant Child Dev (2021) 30:e2241. doi:10.1002/icd.2241

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. O’Regan, D, Jackson, ML, Young, AH, and Rosenzweig, I. Understanding the Impact of the COVID-19 Pandemic, Lockdowns and Social Isolation on Sleep Quality. Nat Sci Sleep (2021) 13:2053–64. doi:10.2147/NSS.S266240

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Orben, A, Tomova, L, and Blakemore, S-J. The Effects of Social Deprivation on Adolescent Development and Mental Health. Lancet Child Adolesc Health (2020) 4(8):634–40. doi:10.1016/s2352-4642(20)30186-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Numbers, K, and Brodaty, H. The Effects of the COVID-19 Pandemic on People with Dementia. Nat Rev Neurol (2021) 17(2):69–70. doi:10.1038/s41582-020-00450-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Thorisdottir, IE, Asgeirsdottir, BB, Kristjansson, AL, Valdimarsdottir, HB, Jonsdottir Tolgyes, EM, Sigfusson, J, et al. Depressive Symptoms, Mental Wellbeing, and Substance Use Among Adolescents before and during the COVID-19 Pandemic in Iceland: A Longitudinal, Population-Based Study. Lancet Psychiatry (2021) 8(8):663–72. doi:10.1016/s2215-0366(21)00156-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Kontoangelos, K, Economou, M, and Papageorgiou, C. Mental Health Effects of COVID-19 Pandemia: A Review of Clinical and Psychological Traits. Psychiatry Investig (2020) 17(6):491–505. doi:10.30773/pi.2020.0161

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Specht, IO, Rohde, JF, Nielsen, AK, Larsen, SC, and Heitmann, BL. Changes in Emotional-Behavioral Functioning Among Pre-school Children Following the Initial Stage Danish COVID-19 Lockdown and Home Confinement. Front Psychol (2021) 12:643057. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2021.643057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Cantiani, C, Dondena, C, Capelli, E, Riboldi, EM, Molteni, M, and Riva, V. Effects of COVID-19 Lockdown on the Emotional and Behavioral Profiles of Preschool Italian Children with and without Familial Risk for Neurodevelopmental Disorders. Brain Sci (2021) 11(4):477. doi:10.3390/brainsci11040477

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Lanfranchi, S, Rea, M, Vianello, R, and Ferri, R. GRIFFITHS III - Griffiths Scales of Child Development: Part II Manual: Administration and Scoring. Italian Edition. 2nd ed. Florence: Hogrefe (2017).

Google Scholar

16. Maione, D, Lucaccioni, L, Arletti, M, Panciroli, G, Poluzzi, F, Ferrari, A, et al. Child Growth, Neurodevelopment and Pre and Postnatal Exposure to Phthalates: First Exposure Data. Eur J Public Health (2020) 30:890. doi:10.1093/eurpub/ckaa166.966

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Mutapi, F, Pfavayi, L, Osakunor, D, Lim, R, Kasambala, M, Mutemeri, A, et al. Assessing Early Child Development and its Association with Stunting and Schistosome Infections in Rural Zimbabwean Children Using the Griffiths Scales of Child Development. Plos Negl Trop Dis (2021) 15(8):e0009660. doi:10.1371/journal.pntd.0009660

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Cirelli, I, Bickle Graz, M, and Tolsa, J-F. Comparison of Griffiths-II and Bayley-II Tests for the Developmental Assessment of High-Risk Infants. Infant Behav Develop (2015) 41:17–25. doi:10.1016/j.infbeh.2015.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Nampijja, M, Webb, E, Nanyunja, C, Sadoo, S, Nalugya, R, Nyonyintono, J, et al. Randomised Controlled Pilot Feasibility Trial of an Early Intervention Programme for Young Infants with Neurodevelopmental Impairment in Uganda: A Study Protocol. BMJ Open (2019) 9(10):e032705. doi:10.1136/bmjopen-2019-032705

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Lanfranchi, S, Rea, M, Vianello, R, and Ferri, R. GRIFFITHS III - Griffiths Scales of Child Development: Part III Manual: Italian Standards. Girls and Boys. Italian Edition. 2nd ed. Florence: Hogrefe (2019).

Google Scholar

21. Griffiths, R. THE ABILITIES OF BABIES. University of London Press (1954). p. 229.

Google Scholar

23. Franco, A, Holý, D, Erhartova, J, Gallo, F, Scalisi, P, and Lorè, B. Final Report of the ESSnet on the Harmonisation and Implementation of a European Socio-Economic Classification: European Socio-Economic Groups (ESeG). [Internet]. Paris: Insee (2014). p. 58. Available from: https://circabc.europa.eu/sd/a/519eafb9-186c-4e2c-a178-902f28501ba4/DSS-2014-Sep-08c%20ESSnet-ESeG%20-%20Final%20Report.pdf (Report No.: DG75-F001).

Google Scholar

24. McBride, BA, and Mills, G. A Comparison of Mother and Father Involvement with Their Preschool Age Children. Early Child Res Q (1993) 8(4):457–77. doi:10.1016/s0885-2006(05)80080-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Cleveland, WS, and Devlin, SJ. Locally Weighted Regression: An Approach to Regression Analysis by Local Fitting. J Am Stat Assoc (1988) 83(403):596–610. doi:10.1080/01621459.1988.10478639

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Provenzi, L, Grumi, S, Altieri, L, Bensi, G, Bertazzoli, E, Biasucci, G, et al. Prenatal Maternal Stress during the COVID-19 Pandemic and Infant Regulatory Capacity at 3 Months: A Longitudinal Study. Dev Psychopathol (2021) 1–9. doi:10.1017/s0954579421000766

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Bennett, DA. How Can I deal with Missing Data in My Study? Aust N Z J Public Health (2001) 25(5):464–9. doi:10.1111/j.1467-842x.2001.tb00294.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Schafer, JL. Multiple Imputation: A Primer. Stat Methods Med Res (1999) 8(1):3–15. doi:10.1191/096228099671525676

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. White, IR, Royston, P, and Wood, AM. Multiple Imputation Using Chained Equations: Issues and Guidance for Practice. Statist Med (2011) 30(4):377–99. doi:10.1002/sim.4067

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30.IBM Corp. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 27.0. [Internet]. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp (2021). Available from: https://www.ibm.com/it-it/analytics/spss-statistics-software.

Google Scholar

31. von Elm, E, Altman, DG, Egger, M, Pocock, SJ, Gøtzsche, PC, and Vandenbroucke, JP. The Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE) Statement: Guidelines for Reporting Observational Studies. Lancet (2007) 370(9596):1453–7. doi:10.1016/s0140-6736(07)61602-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32.The Lancet Psychiatry. COVID-19 and Mental Health. Lancet Psychiatry (2021) 8(2):87. doi:10.1016/S2215-0366(21)00005-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Torales, J, O’Higgins, M, Castaldelli-Maia, JM, and Ventriglio, A. The Outbreak of COVID-19 Coronavirus and its Impact on Global Mental Health. Int J Soc Psychiatry (2020) 66(4):317–20. doi:10.1177/0020764020915212

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Kestel, D, Dua, T, Hanna, F, and Castro, J. The Impact of COVID-19 on Mental, Neurological and Substance Use Services. [Internet]. Geneva: WHO (2022). Available from: https://www.who.int/publications-detail-redirect/978924012455.

Google Scholar

35.Istat.it. Popolazione per condizione professionale: Tutte le età. [Internet] (2022). Available from: http://dati.istat.it/Index.aspx?QueryId=42542.

Google Scholar

36.Regione Emilia-Romagna. Statistica, Popolazione. [Internet] (2022). Available from: https://statistica.regione.emilia-romagna.it/servizi-online/statistica-self-service/popolazione.

Google Scholar

37. Deoni, SC, Beauchemin, J, Volpe, A, and Dâ Sa, VRESONANCE Consortium. Impact of the COVID-19 Pandemic on Early Child Cognitive Development: Initial Findings in a Longitudinal Observational Study of Child Health. medRxiv (2021). doi:10.1101/2021.08.10.21261846

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Johnson, CP, and Blasco, PA. Infant Growth and Development. Pediatr Rev (1997) 18(7):224–42. doi:10.1542/pir.18-7-224

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Orgilés, M, Morales, A, Delvecchio, E, Mazzeschi, C, and Espada, JP. Immediate Psychological Effects of the COVID-19 Quarantine in Youth from Italy and Spain. Front Psychol (2020) 11:579038. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2020.579038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Perrin, EC, Leslie, LK, and Boat, T. Parenting as Primary Prevention. JAMA Pediatr (2016) 170(7):637–8. doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2016.0225

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Oliva-Arnanz, A, Romay-Barrero, H, Romero-Galisteo, R-P, Pinero-Pinto, E, Lirio-Romero, C, and Palomo-Carrión, R. Families' Perceptions of the Motor Development and Quality of Life of Their Children Aged 0-3 Years during Home Confinement Due to the COVID-19 Pandemic. A Descriptive Study. Children (2021) 8(12):1149. doi:10.3390/children8121149

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Moore, T, Arefadib, N, Deery, A, and West, S. The First Thousand Days: An Evidence Paper. [Internet]. Parkville, Victoria: Centre for Community Child Health, Murdoch Children’s Research Institute (2017). Available from: https://apo.org.au/node/108431.

Google Scholar

Keywords: public health, SARS-CoV-2, infant, Griffiths development scales, child development, mental processes, psychomotor performance, physical distancing

Citation: Ferrari E, Palandri L, Lucaccioni L, Talucci G, Passini E, Trevisani V and Righi E (2022) The Kids Are Alright (?). Infants’ Development and COVID-19 Pandemic: A Cross-Sectional Study. Int J Public Health 67:1604804. doi: 10.3389/ijph.2022.1604804

Received: 28 January 2022; Accepted: 13 May 2022;
Published: 20 June 2022.

Edited by:

Franco Mascayano, New York State Psychiatric Institute (NYSPI), United States

Reviewed by:

Joanna Mazur, University of Zielona Góra, Poland

Copyright © 2022 Ferrari, Palandri, Lucaccioni, Talucci, Passini, Trevisani and Righi. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Lucia Palandri, palandri.lucia@gmail.com

These authors have contributed equally to this work and share first authorship

This Original Article is part of the IJPH Special Issue “The Impact of the COVID-19 Pandemic on Mental Health”

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.