LETTER TO THE EDITOR

Int J Public Health, 02 June 2025

Volume 70 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/ijph.2025.1608711

Bullying Trends or Definitional Drift? A Methodological Critique of Molcho et al.’s Study

  • Sociodicy Institute, Corbenay, France

To the editor,

Molcho et al. recently investigated temporal trends in bullying using data from the Health Behaviour in School-aged Children (HBSC) survey [1]. The authors analyzed the evolution of in-school bullying (1994–2022) and cyberbullying (2018–2022) prevalence. Results were indicative of a decrease in in-school bullying perpetration and victimization and of an increase in cyberbullying perpetration and victimization. While I commend the authors’ efforts to provide insights into temporal trends in bullying, I have two main concerns about the validity of their findings.

First, while the authors acknowledged that cyberbullying is an ill-defined construct [[1], p. 2], they omitted to mention that the same applies to in-school or so-called traditional bullying [25]. Notably, the authors did not clarify how (cyber)bullying has been defined in the HBSC study and did not reproduce the preamble outlining bullying in each HBSC survey. It appears, however, that this preamble has changed over time. As an illustration, in 1998, this preamble stated:

We say a student is being bullied when another student, or a group of students, say or do nasty and unpleasant things to him or her. It is also bullying when a student is teased repeatedly in a way he or she doesn’t like. But it is not bullying when two students of about the same strength quarrel or fight [[6], p. 2095].

I note that the reference to repetitiveness is made in the second sentence in relation to teasing. In 2018, the preamble stated:

We say a person is being bullied when another person or a group of people, repeatedly say or do unwanted nasty and unpleasant things to him or her. It also is bullying when a person is teased in a way he or she does not like or when he or she is left out of things on purpose. The person that bullies has more power than the person being bullied and wants to cause harm to him or her. It is not bullying when two people of about the same strength or power argue or fight [[7], p. 2].

Here, the reference to repetitiveness is made in the first sentence and no longer relates to teasing. This modification involved a different conception of bullying that may partly account for some observed changes in the prevalence of in-school bullying victimization and perpetration. In effect, having nasty and unpleasant things said or done to someone repeatedly over the past couple of months may occur less frequently than a single instance of such an event. In addition, in contrast to its 2018 version, the preamble used in 1998 mentioned neither social bullying (i.e., being “left out of things on purpose”) nor bully’s intent to harm. Coupled with the changes, reported by the authors [[1], p. 3], applied to the response rating scales, these definitional revisions question the diachronic comparability of the data examined in this study.

Second, whereas the authors aimed to examine temporal trends in in-school bullying perpetration and victimization, they excluded from their analyses the participants reporting either having been bullied or having bullied schoolmates once or twice in the past couple of months. The authors justified their use of this categorization criterion by (a) referring to a study by Sollberg and Olweus [8] and (b) claiming that doing so permitted them to “capture a regular pattern of perpetration [and victimization]” [[1], p. 3]. I do not think these are sound arguments. Indeed, Sollberg and Olweus used a definitional preamble in which repetitiveness was not a necessary dimension of bullying but only a potential characteristic of the phenomenon [[8], p. 246]. Because the preamble used in the most recent version of the HBSC survey attributes to repetitiveness an essential role, both frameworks should not be considered as conveying the same definition of bullying. For instance, reporting having been insulted once can refer to a one-off event within Sollberg and Olweus’ framework; by contrast, the “same” report refers to one sequence of repeated insults within the HBSC study’s framework. Unfortunately, the HBSC survey assesses neither the number of repetitions nor the time scope of the sequence(s) in question. In sum, invoking Sollberg and Olweus’ study is unwarranted, since Molcho et al. relied on a different conceptual and methodological design; claiming that the categorization criterion employed allowed the authors to “capture regular patterns” is misleading, since they did not assess the number of repetitions and/or the duration of the entire bullying sequence(s).

Considered altogether, these two points question the validity of the study findings. Temporal trend analysis requires the use of consistent assessment methods over time. Crucially, Molcho et al.’s study does not meet this basic requirement. Moreover, the exclusion of the participants reporting having been bullied or having bullied schoolmates once or twice in the past couple of months rests on problematic theoretical and methodological grounds. While including these participants might only marginally impact the overall trends, it would significantly affect prevalence estimates. I therefore urge readers to interpret the study findings with caution and advise against adopting the same categorization criterion in future research using data from the HBSC survey.

Author Contributions

RB, as the sole author, is responsible for all aspects of the letter’s content.

Funding

The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article.

Conflict of Interest

The author declares that they do not have any conflicts of interest.

Generative AI Statement

The authors declare that no Generative AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.

References

1. Molcho, M, Walsh, SD, King, N, Pickett, W, Donnelly, PD, Cosma, A, et al. Trends in Indicators of Violence Among Adolescents in Europe and North America 1994–2022. Int J Public Health (2025) 70:1607654. doi:10.3389/ijph.2025.1607654

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Smith, PK. Understanding School Bullying: Its Nature and Prevention Strategies. London: Sage (2014).

Google Scholar

3. Volk, AA, Veenstra, R, and Espelage, DL. So You Want to Study Bullying? Recommendations to Enhance the Validity, Transparency, and Compatibility of Bullying Research. Aggress Violent Behav (2017) 36:34–43. doi:10.1016/j.avb.2017.07.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Canty, J, Stubbe, M, Steers, D, and Collings, S. The Trouble with Bullying – Deconstructing the Conventional Definition of Bullying for a Child-Centred Investigation into Children's Use of Social Media. Child Soc (2016) 30(1):48–58. doi:10.1111/chso.12103

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Chang, V. Inconsistent Definitions of Bullying: A Need to Examine People’s Judgments and Reasoning about Bullying and Cyberbullying. Hum Dev (2021) 65(3):144–59. doi:10.1159/000516838

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Nansel, TR, Overpeck, M, Pilla, RS, Ruan, WJ, Simons-Morton, B, and Scheidt, P. Bullying Behaviors Among US Youth: Prevalence and Association with Psychosocial Adjustment. JAMA (2001) 285(16):2094–100. doi:10.1001/jama.285.16.2094

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Torchyan, AA, Houkes, I, and Bosma, H. Income Inequality and Socioeconomic Differences in Bullying Perpetration Among Adolescents in Post-Communist Countries of Europe: Findings from the HBSC Study. Prev Med Rep (2024) 37:102540. doi:10.1016/j.pmedr.2023.102540

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Solberg, ME, and Olweus, D. Prevalence Estimation of School Bullying with the Olweus Bully/Victim Questionnaire. Aggress Behav (2003) 29(3):239–68. doi:10.1002/ab.10047

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: bullying, temporal trends, definitional inconsistency, methodological considerations, HBSC study

Citation: Brisson R (2025) Bullying Trends or Definitional Drift? A Methodological Critique of Molcho et al.’s Study. Int. J. Public Health 70:1608711. doi: 10.3389/ijph.2025.1608711

Received: 16 May 2025; Accepted: 20 May 2025;
Published: 02 June 2025.

Edited by:

Andrea Madarasova Geckova, University of Pavol Jozef Šafárik, Slovakia

Copyright © 2025 Brisson. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Romain Brisson, c29jaW9kaWN5QG91dGxvb2suY29t

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.